UNDERSTANDING THE PIECES THAT MAKE UP THE BIOLOGY PUZZLE

^

SCIENCE

A science is a body of facts systematically arranged and operating according to general laws.

 

^

SUBJECTIVE / OBJECTIVE

In biology, we can approach the same problem either subjectively or objectively – depending on our theory structure.

^

SUBJECTIVE

Consider clinical diagnosis wherein a physician must distinguish between disorders carrying similar symptoms.  Bipolar and borderline personality disorders offer such an example.

^

Symptoms offer subjective measures.

^

OBJECTIVE

Alternatively, we can elect an objective approach by translating the MRI data of patients into quantitative patterns that show the relationship of parts to connections.

^

Dots represent disorders and mathematical markers, whereas blue lines identify connections.

^

OBJECTIVE

In turn, we can replace the dots with the names of the disorders and mathematical markers they represent.  Right click the image to magnify.

^

The book explains how databases populated with such information can become powerful diagnostic and predictive tools.

^

THEORY STRUCTURE

A theory structure defines the rules.

 

^

It determines what our data can do.

^

REDUCTIONIST THEORY

Reductionist theory sees biology as a collection of disconnected parts.

^

By taking biology apart, it delivers the parts, but not the connections.  Make no mistake, it assumes that we can play the biology game successfully with only half the information.  When applied to biology, which is a complexity, such an assumption produces a 50% science.

^

Unfortunately, a 50% science is often no science at all – as the story will show.

^

COMPLEXITY THEORY

Complexity theory sees the same set of parts as a highly connected set.

^

With both parts and connections in play, biology can be explored as a complexity consisting of mathematical puzzles.

^

The book explains how to set up and solve such puzzles.

^

PHENOTYPE

Progress in biology depends importantly on our ability to understand the complex relationship of genotype to phenotype.

 

^

The book focuses on the phenotype because it is – far and away – the best source of complexity.

^

BIOLOGY

A phenotype is the translation of a genome into a complex organism.

^

Biology performs this translation the easy way.

Right click the image to magnify.

^

BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES

We prefer to do it the hard way.

^

We get to deal with two complexities.  One produced by biology and and a second one produced by our theory structure – reductionism.

^

STATISTICIANS

Statisticians report that we can expect our published results to be correct only about 20 to 30% of the time.  Apparently, research data are being overwhelmed by the biases coming from our experimental methods.  If true, this is embarrassing because we are being bested by a simple coin toss.

^

RESEARCH DATA

Obviously, something is sadly amiss.

^

GOING TO THE SOURCE

We begin by considering three sources of noise in our data – bias, biological variation, and valence.

^

DEFINITION

A systematic distortion in data produces bias.  It limits our ability to detect differences, or, in the case of biology, changes.

^

A 50% EFFORT

Recall that reductionist theory allows us to look at only some of the data, the parts but not the connections.

^

SOURCES OF BIAS

Practically everything we do in the lab introduces bias.  Consider, for example, the problem of counting molecules.

^

COUNTING MOLECULES - WITHOUT COMPLEXITY

Since the number of molecules in the control and experimental samples is the same, the %Change = 100%.

^

COUNTING MOLECULES - WITHOUT COMPLEXITY

If we run an experiment and discover that 83% of the molecules remain (compared to the control), we conclude that the number of molecules decreased by 17%.

^

COUNTING MOLECULES - WITH COMPLEXITY

Reconnect the molecules to the cells, however, and we see that the decrease in the concentration of molecules was produced by a slight increase in the cell volume.  The number of molecules remained constant.  Right click the image to magnify.

^

DEFINITION

Biological variation refers to the amount a measure of a specific part can vary.  The larger the spread, the harder it is to detect a significant difference between two sets of measurements.

 

^

It helps to make biological data noisy.

^

BIOLOGICAL VARIATION

Consider the amygdala of the human brain, which consists of a left and right component.  When measured as isolated parts, the volumes display a wide range of values.

^

BIOLOGICAL VARIATION - GOING...

If, however, we plot the volume of the left side against that of the right, we find that all the points conveniently sit on the same line.

^

BIOLOGICAL VARIATION - ...GOING...

Next, if we express the volumes of left to right sides as ratios, the points continue to sit on the line with a surprising narrow range of values.

^

BIOLOGICAL VARIATION - ...GONE.

Finally, if we express all the left to right volume ratios as decimal repertoire values, all the points condense to form a single value.  In effect, forming ratios of biological values minimizes variation.  By taking its directions from biology, complexity theory makes difficult puzzles easy to solve.

^

DEFINITION

The quantity that determines the proportion of one part to another; an ability of a given part to connect to the same part in different ratios.

^

Biology defines and controls its complexity by using ratios of one part to another.

^

VALENCE - POSTMORTEM

The ratio of the nucleus to a cell organelle can vary widely within and between cell types.

^

VALENCE - POSTMORTEM

A blueprint reveals the range of the valences within and across species.

^

VALENCE - IN LIVING SUBJECTS

The human hippocampus, for example, displays multiple valences – differing more than a factor of two.

^

UNIVERSAL DATA

The primary role of data in a reductionist model is to detect local biological changes, whereas that of a complexity model is to use the same data to solve complex problems.

 

^

Data become universal when they share the same format and can interact mathematically.

^

EXTRACTING DATA

Since a surprisingly large proportion of published data exist as graphs, assembling a universal data set involves capturing data from figures.  Interactive screens, such as the one shown, simplify this process.

^

CATALOGING DATA

Data transferred to relational databases can be stored hierarchically, …

^

SORTING & FILTERING DATA

… viewed in data tables, …

^

TRANSFORMING DATA

…formed into ratios, …

^

WORKING DATA

… and used to solve specific problems (e.g., diagnosis).

^

INFORMATION

When data exist in databases, they can create new forms of information.

 

^

In effect, databases redefine our relationship to published data.

^

TRANSFORMING

Although they start out as a simple catalog of facts, data can generate complex patterns by forming connections.  This transformation consists of forming ratios of one part to another.

^

GROWING

A relatively small number of original data points taken from a single publication can become large numbers of patterns when expressed as data pairs, triplets, and quadruplets.  Such strings can serve as mathematical markers.

^

CONTRIBUTING

When interpreted as mathematical markers and held in a data cage, for example, global data sets can diagnose disorders of the brain with a reliably of 100%.

^

BIOLOGICAL CHANGE

A simple approach to biology looks for changes in data points, whereas a complex approach looks for changes in patterns.

 

^

SIMULATIONS

When we look at the behavior of parts theoretically, we find evidence of their interdependence.  This means that we can expect to get many different answers to the same question.

^

DATA TRAPS

Research data offer confirmation in that we can find many examples where concentrations and absolute values fail to agree.  Values highlighted in red signal an increase, whereas those highlighted in blue a decrease.

^

PATTERNS - BEFORE

In a complexity, such as the human brain, a normal phenotype displays a pattern with one set of connections, but…

^

PATTERNS - AFTER

…quite a different pattern with different connections when a disorder exists.

^

PATTERNS - IN N-DIMENSIONAL SPACE

The same data can be viewed and rotated in three dimensions, or …

^

PATTERNS - AS EQUATIONS

…expressed as equations to quantify the differences that exist between normal (blue) and schizophrenic (red) brains.

^

REPRODUCIBILITY

The touchstone of a quantitative science is an ability to reproduce results.

^

Biology – operating as a 50% science – struggles with the reproducible problem .

^

REPRODUCIBILITY - LOCAL - POSTMORTEM

Stereological data – collected postmortem – display such a capability locally (within a given paper), but…

^

REPRODUCIBILITY - GLOBAL - POSTMORTEM

…not often globally (between papers) for the same species.

^

REPRODUCIBILITY - GLOBAL - LIVING

A very different outcome exists for data collected from living subjects under the auspices of complexity theory.  Now reproducibility becomes commonplace – both locally and globally in the same species.

^

DISTORTIONS - TEST

When biological data sets fail to display global reproducibility, they most likely carry distortions (biases) produced by our experimental methods.

 

^

DISTORTIONS VS GOLD STANDARDS

We can test this idea by using mathematical markers for schizophrenia coming from living (MRI) and postmortem (stereology) subjects.

^

Using the MRI data as standards, can we diagnose patients correctly using data collected postmortem (stereology)?

^

No, such a test misses the correct diagnosis (SCHIZOPHRENIA) by a wide margin.

^

DISTORTIONS - ARE HIGHLY VARIABLE

The postmortem data carry volume distortions, including increases (swelling) and decreases (shrinkage).

^

DISTORTIONS - UNDER CONTROL

Once corrections are applied, however, the postmortem markers deliver the correct diagnosis.

^

DISORDERS OF THE BRAIN

Disorders of the human brain affect many of the same parts.

^

DISORDERS OF THE BRAIN - QUALITATIVE PATTERNS

Schizophrenia, for example, shares many of its parts with a host of other disorders.

^

DISORDERS OF THE BRAIN - QUALITATIVE PATTERNS

In fact, the tendency of disorders to share parts is a distinct feature of the disease process.

^

DISORDERS OF THE BRAIN - QUANTITATIVE PATTERNS

Different disorders often share exactly the same mathematical markers.

^

Notice that Down’s syndrome shares most of its markers with schizophrenia.

^

DISORDERS OF THE BRAIN - QUANTITATIVE PATTERNS

By replacing the dots with mathematical markers and disorders, we can see the quantitative relationship of one phenotype (Down’s syndrome) to another (schizophrenia).

^

COMMUNITIES

Disorders of the human brain form communities of shared markers.

 

^

IDENTIFY THE DATA SET

If we start with 21 well-defined disorders, phenotype each one with mathematical markers, and then mix all the markers together, we end up with a first approximation of what constitutes a disordered brain.

^

BEGIN THE UNFOLDING

From this mixture, communities of disorders will form according to the strength of their affinities.

This is how the clinical literature appears when expressed as a complexity (parts and connections) and designed to discover how disorders of the brain are related.

^

The first analysis finds five communities, …

^

CONTINUE THE UNFOLDING

…which, in turn, yield progressively smaller communities…

^

FINISH THE UNFOLDING

…wherein only pairs of disorders remain.

^

COMMUNITIES DEFINE BIG DATA GAMES

Finally, if we plot a small set of disorders with their markers, the magnitude of biological complexity as it exists in the human brain becomes apparent.

^

DIAGNOSIS AND PREDICTION

Diagnosis and prediction differ only by time.

 

 

^

DATA CAGE

At the left is a data cage designed for the human brain.  It includes a collection of twenty-six disorders – each of which exists in the figure as a central point surrounded by a set of unique mathematical markers.  Notice that by eliminating the many connections that normally exist among such disorders, we effectively eliminate all the noise that would otherwise be coming from false positives.  For convenience, the example is limited to 50,000 markers – out of 245,000.

This is what the clinical literature looks like when expressed as a complexity and converted into a diagnostic tool.

^

DATA CAGE IN ACTION

To illustrate the effectiveness of the data cage as a diagnostic tool, we can copy twenty markers from the same disorder, rename them as unknowns, and then plot the data a second time.  The new plot shows that all the “unknowns” connect to a single disorder – the one from which they were originally copied (bipolar disorder).  In effect, a data cage provides a quantitative standard to which unknowns can be compared – be they internal or external.  In practice, the diagnostic test is surprisingly easy to run.  It consists of adding unknowns to the database (245,000), identifying duplicates, and making the diagnosis (unknowns=knowns).  It takes just a few minutes to run.

The point to take from the example is that our task in learning how to diagnose disorders as complex systems begins by first understanding the basic properties of the system in play.  Since we now know that different disorders of the human brain often share similar mathematical markers, creating data cages to remove this overwhelming influx of false positives becomes a logical first step in preparing for a clinical diagnosis.  We also know that large data sets derived from biology suffer grievously from a host of problems, which include experimental bias, biological variation, and false positives.  The advantage of complexity theory is that it helps us to understand the root causes of these problems so that we can move confidently toward viable solutions.  Think of it this way.  If we know what’s broken, we can figure out how to fix it.

^

REPRODUCIBILITY TEST - REPORT 2016

Giving the job to biology…

^

TEST APPLIED TO THE LITERATURE

Most would agree that an ability to repeat the results of an experiment is fundamental to scientific discovery.  Reproducibility, however, can be identified with precision or accuracy, the choice being left to the theory structure.  Reductionist theory typically chooses precision (repeatability), whereas complexity theory prefers accuracy (correctness).  To develop a test for reproducibility that can be applied to several published studies simultaneously, accuracy proved to be the best choice because the problem became a complex one.  The test consisted of working backwards from global patterns (biology literature) to reproducible patterns (mathematical markers vs. connection ratios) to accuracy (biology).  When applied to the MRI data of patients (67 studies), reproducibility was found to exist in surprisingly large amounts – as shown here and explained in the report (2016).

^

THE HOMOGENEITY POSTULATES - REPORT 2017

Test biochemistry first and then biology.

^

RULES STATED AS EQUATIONS

When morphological and biochemical data points fit a linear regression with an R2 = 1, the resulting equation identifies a biological rule with the ability to predict either structure or function. We can use such rules to play complexity games wherein we pose questions and biology predicts answers. In theory, this approach should allow us to reverse the direction of time so that we can discover where and how the rules are stored, coded, and read.